Skip to main content

Trade-offs between immunity and competitive ability in fighting ant males

Abstract

Background

Fighting disease while fighting rivals exposes males to constraints and trade-offs during male-male competition. We here tested how both the stage and intensity of infection with the fungal pathogen Metarhizium robertsii interfere with fighting success in Cardiocondyla obscurior ant males. Males of this species have evolved long lifespans during which they can gain many matings with the young queens of the colony, if successful in male-male competition. Since male fights occur inside the colony, the outcome of male-male competition can further be biased by interference of the colony’s worker force.

Results

We found that severe, but not yet mild, infection strongly impaired male fighting success. In late-stage infection, this could be attributed to worker aggression directed towards the infected rather than the healthy male and an already very high male morbidity even in the absence of fighting. Shortly after pathogen exposure, however, male mortality was particularly increased during combat. Since these males mounted a strong immune response, their reduced fighting success suggests a trade-off between immune investment and competitive ability already early in the infection. Even if the males themselves showed no difference in the number of attacks they raised against their healthy rivals across infection stages and levels, severely infected males were thus losing in male-male competition from an early stage of infection on.

Conclusions

Males of the ant C. obscurior have a well-developed immune system that raises a strong immune response very fast after fungal exposure. This allows them to cope with mild pathogen exposures without compromising their success in male-male competition, and hence to gain multiple mating opportunities with the emerging virgin queens of the colony. Under severe infection, however, they are weak fighters and rarely survive a combat already at early infection when raising an immune response, as well as at progressed infection, when they are morbid and preferentially targeted by worker aggression. Workers thereby remove males that pose a future disease threat by biasing male-male competition. Our study thus reveals a novel social immunity mechanism how social insect workers protect the colony against disease risk.

Peer Review reports

Background

Escalated male fighting over access to females is common among many vertebrate species [1], and can also occur in invertebrates, like cephalopods [2], butterflies [3], parasitoid and fig wasps [4, 5], as well as some ant species [6,7,8,9,10,11]. Despite the high costs that fights can bear for both competitors in terms of e.g. time consumption and risk of injury, even lethal combats evolve when it allows the winning males to monopolize mating with the females [1, 12].

Male fighting ability relies on physical strength, which often becomes compromised when males get sick. Mounting an immune response to fight an infection requires resources that cannot simultaneously be invested into fighting rivals, leading to a classic trade-off situation. Under infection, reallocation of resources away from growth and reproduction is therefore commonly observed already at early stages of infection like the incubation period before typical disease symptoms appear [13,14,15]. When disease has progressed, hosts often even show extensive “sickness behaviours” like lethargy and disengagement from most social interactions [16,17,18,19,20], thereby conserving energy to fight off the infection [21,22,23,24]. However, refraining oneself from engaging in male-male competition when sick does not seem to be a good strategy for males that will not gain a later chance for reproduction. This is the case either when disease will end deadly, or when hosts are so short-lived that reproduction cannot be postponed to after they have overcome the disease [25]. Under such conditions, males have nothing to lose and often even fight more violently against a rival, as a “terminal investment” strategy [26]. Due to these apparent trade-offs, short-lived species typically do not invest many resources into a functional immune system, and in longer-lived species, immunity is often reduced during the reproductive period (also due to a direct immunosuppressive effect of the male hormone testosterone) [16,17,18,19,20]. Moreover, increased irritability and aggression are common behavioural reactions to disease in vertebrates, including humans [21, 27], which can also be found in insects, including ants [28]. Therefore, it is not straightforward to predict the effect of infection on male fighting ability, particularly since it will also be affected by (i) the level of infection, i.e. if it represents a low-level infection that causes mild or no disease symptoms, or a high-level infection inducing severe disease and death, and (ii) on its stage after exposure, i.e. whether the male suffers from progressed infection or is still in the early incubation period mounting up its immune response.

When fighting occurs in a social context, it may be further influenced by conspecifics. For example, the workers of social insects would have the potential to bias the outcome of male-male competition, if it takes place inside the colony. Such intra-nest fighting has evolved in several ant species in the genera Cardiocondyla and Hypoponera, when mating occurs in the maternal nest and emerging queen numbers are small enough to be monopolized by the winner male [6,7,8, 10, 12, 29]. Whilst it is not yet known if such worker manipulation of male fights exists, it was found that ant workers can affect sexual selection by choosing or preferring some males as the queen’s partners [30,31,32]. As workers are sterile but gain inclusive fitness via the reproduction of their related queens and males, such interference with mate choice has been referred to as “inclusive sexual selection” [31]. Biasing the outcome of male-male competition would similarly allow workers to favour some males over others as the queens’ future mates. In addition, worker aggression towards infected males could represent a form of social immunity [33]. By helping the healthy male to win the fight, the diseased rival, who poses an infection risk to the colony, would be eliminated, reducing the danger of a disease outbreak in the colony. Such bias introduced by workers on male-male competition would therefore correspond to an ‘inclusive form’ of the Hamilton-Zuk hypothesis of parasite-mediated sexual selection [19, 34].

We used the ant Cardiocondyla obscurior as a model system to test for the occurrence of worker interference with male-male competition in ants, and its possible interplay with male disease state. As typical for invasive ants [35,36,37], the queens and males of C. obscurior mate inside the nest [8, 29, 38]. The wingless fighter males patrol the nest to identify emerging virgin queens to mate with, as well as emerging rival males to attack and engage in lethal combats. During a fight, males grab and besmear the rival male with a hindgut secretion that attracts workers, which then bite the besmeared male to death [39, 40]. Besmearing is typically performed by the older and hence dominant male, yet fights between similarly-aged rivals often involve mutual grabbing and besmearing attacks [40], which will give workers the opportunity to bias the fight outcome.

Successful males that are able to quickly detect emerging rivals thus can gain access to virgin queens over a long period of multiple weeks and can perform up to 50 and more matings [41]. This has led to the evolution of both, a prolonged lifespan comparable to a worker’s lifespan, as well as a lifelong spermatogenesis in these fighter males [8, 42]. C. obscurior fighter males therefore greatly differ from ‘typical’ ant males, which are very short-lived, have completed their spermatogenesis before adult emergence and only have a single mating opportunity per lifetime in a mating flight, during which they engage in scramble competition with the other males [9, 12, 43]. We would thus expect C. obscurior males to invest in a functioning immune system – in contrast to other social insect males with reported low immunocompetence [44,45,46]. Yet, this could make them vulnerable to an immunity-reproduction trade-off.

We therefore observed how exposure of C. obscurior males to the fungal pathogen Metarhizium robertsii – a common natural fungal pathogen of ants [47,48,49] – affects the male’s own fighting ability and the workers’ interference in fights between a healthy and an infected male. To test for a possible trade-off between immunity and fighting ability we either exposed males to a low or a high dose of the fungal pathogen. Since the outcome of Metarhizium infections are dose-dependent [50, 51], the low dose was chosen to induce only a mild infection that the male’s immune system was expected to be able to cope with, whilst the high dose should in most cases cause severe disease, leading to death. For both, low and high dose, we tested the fighting success of infected males when in a combat situation with a healthy male (i) early in the infection, i.e. directly after exposure, when the pathogen’s infectious stages (the conidiospores, hereafter abbreviated as spores) start penetrating the host’s cuticle, and (ii) at a progressed infection stage two days after exposure [52]. We expected that not only severe disease but already mounting an early immune response may negatively impact the males’ fighting ability. Apart from that, since the males face both a deadly disease and a deadly combat situation, they should show terminal investment of all their (remaining) resources into fighting. We also predicted that – if workers would indeed bias fight outcome – they should discriminate against the infected male, either to prevent pathogen spread by contagious males (early after exposure) or because the males represent a future infection risk to the colony by developing into a sporulating cadaver (when fighting at later stage of disease).

Results

Severe infections reduce male fighting success from an early disease stage

C. obscurior males that had been exposed to a high pathogen load of M. robertsii had a reduced fighting success, when in a 24 h-long combat situation with a healthy male (exposed to a pathogen-free sham treatment; control male). This was equally so for the early stage of infection, when males were put into the fight situation immediately after pathogen exposure (Fig. 1a), and when their infection had already progressed to a later stage, as males had been exposed already 48 h before the start of the fight (Fig. 1b). Over all 347 fights (Table S1), we observed three different fight outcomes after 24 h. These were that fights either remained undecided, with both males either being alive or both males being dead, or that they ended as ‘decided fights’ with one rival surviving and the other being killed in combat (for the frequency of these outcomes see pie charts in Fig. 1a,b; for statistical comparisons see Table S2). Notably, compared to fights between two healthy males, fights, in which one of the males was infected with a high dose, more frequently ended with one clear winner and one clear loser (trending in early infection: GLMM, p = 0.059; significant in late infection: p = 0.039; for statistical details see Table S2). Importantly, in the great majority of these decided fights, it was the infected male that lost the competition, independent of the stage of its infection: 85% (34/40) of the early-infected males (Chi-Square test, χ2 = 11.169, df = 1, p < 0.001), and 93.5% (43/46) of the late-infected males (Chi-Square test, χ2 = 21.445 df = 1, p < 0.001) had been killed at the end of the fighting period when exposed to a high pathogen dose (Fig. 1a,b).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Fighting ability of infected males in dependence of infection dose and stage. Fight outcome for infected males compared to their healthy rival males at (a) the early (green) and (b) the late (blue) stage of infection. Pie charts show the proportion of fights that ended undecided with either both males still alive (white), or both males dead (black), or decided with one clear winner and loser (grey; Table S2). In the decided fights (grey offset slice with coloured outline), infected males had a higher risk of losing the fight than by chance (dotted line) after exposure to a high, but not low pathogen dose (bars depict the proportion of fights lost by the infected male; darker tone reflects higher dose; based on a total of 347 fights, detailed in Table S1). The aggression level of infected males (c,d) did not differ from that of their healthy rivals across infection stage or level, whilst workers showed increased aggression only to late-stage high-level infected males (e,f). Individual data points depict aggression by (c,d) and towards (e,f) each infected male compared to its healthy rival (aggressioninfected−healthy, i.e. resulting in a zero value under equal aggression and positive values for higher aggression), boxes give the 95% CI and median as black line (based on 112 observed fights between healthy and infected males). The added risk of dying for infected males when fighting (g,h) compared to their baseline mortality when non-fighting during the same period (Fig. S1; Table S3) was significantly increased for males in the early and late stage of a high-level infection, where their mortality risk during fight increased 2.2-fold at the early stage and 1.4-fold at the late-stage. Bars depict the fold change of mortality during combat to the males’ respective baseline mortality (value of 1 depicts equal mortality). Based on 201 fights. Significant deviation from chance (50:50) (a,b), from the healthy male (c-f) and from their non-fighting baseline mortality (g,h) given for each group. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001, ns = non-significant

When males had only received a low pathogen dose that rarely causes disease, there was no difference between the probability of the fight ending with a clear winner and loser compared to fights between two healthy males, for both infection stages (early: GLMM, p = 0.236; late: p = 0.433; for statistical details see Table S2). Moreover, when fights were decided, low level-infected males did not lose the fights with a higher probability than their healthy rivals (Fisher’s Exact test, early: 4/10, p = 1.000; late: 3/8, p = 1.000; Fig. 1a,b; see also Table S2).

Workers attack severely-infected males at a late disease stage

To test whether the fighting success of infected males could be explained by their own aggression level towards their competitor, or by the aggression they received from the workers, we observed the behaviour of the fighting males and their workers. We found that – independent of dose and stage of infection – infected males showed the same level of aggression towards their rivals as the healthy males did towards them (Fig. 1c,d; GLMMs,

early infection low dose: χ2 = 0.459, p = 0.853 and high dose: χ2 = 0.073, p = 0.787, late infection low dose: χ2 = 0.261, p = 0.609 and high dose: χ2 = 0.178, p = 0.673; all df = 1). This means that even the high-level infected males, who finally lost the fight in the majority of cases (Fig. 1a,b), did not perform fewer attacks. The number of worker attacks towards the infected male compared to its healthy rival was twice as high in fights with high-level exposed males at a late stage of their infection (GLMM, χ2 = 8.155, df = 1, p = 0.009), but not in the early stage (GLMM, χ2 = 1.516, df = 1, p = 0.436), or after low-dose exposure of the males (Fig. 1e,f; GLMMs, early low: χ2 = 0.035, df = 1, p = 0.853; late low: χ2 = 2.599, df = 1, p = 0.214).

Males suffer highest fighting costs early in severe infection

Worker aggression could explain why high-level infected males at a late disease stage frequently lost the fights. However, at the early infection stage, the high-level infected males had equally low fighting success, even if workers did not treat them with increased aggression (Fig. 1a,b,e,f). We therefore determined the baseline mortality risk of the males in their respective fighting period in the absence of a fight, to disentangle, how much of the mortality of fighting males could be contributed to the fight itself, and how much to an already impaired survival due to infection. To this end, individual males were kept in the absence of a competitor with their workers for a 24 h period, starting either directly after pathogen exposure, or after infection had been established for 48 h. Whilst healthy males had a mortality risk of 8–9% (Table S3), infection in general increased this risk. At the early stage of infection, this was only significant for males exposed to a high pathogen dose (low dose: 17%; GLMM, χ2 = 3.323, df = 1, p = 0.091; high dose: 29%; GLMM, χ2 = 8.992, df = 1, p = 0.005). When infection had progressed, the males’ baseline mortality risk in the absence of fighting was already pronounced at the low dose (36%; GLMM, χ2 = 7.624, df = 1, p = 0.010), and reached high morbidity (56% mortality) at the high pathogen dose (GLMM, χ2 = 24.820, df = 1, p < 0.001; Fig. S1; Table S3). Notably, this means that progressed infection led to a high infection-induced baseline mortality, with no significant further increase of mortality caused by fighting at the low dose (Chi-Square test, χ2 = 0.086, df = 1, p = 0.769). At the high dose, however, a 1.4-fold increased mortality was observed (Chi-Square test, χ2 = 7.107, df = 1, p = 0.010, Fig. 1h). Early-infected males showed comparably lower baseline mortality, but died more than two times more frequently during fighting when infected with a high dose (2.2-fold increased mortality; Chi-Square test, χ2 = 16.120, df = 1, p < 0.001; Fig. 1g, but not at the low dose; Fisher’s Exact test, df = 1, p = 0.730). Fighting costs therefore incurred for males in the early and late stage of severe infection, but were much more pronounced for early stage-infected males (Fig. 1g,h).

Severe infection induces a strong early immune response

These data suggest that fighting as an additional stressor may unveil costs of mounting an immune response at the early stage of infection, causing increased mortality when the immune response is strong, as expected under severe but not yet mild infection. We therefore tested if immune genes were activated already within the first 24 h after pathogen exposure, and whether this immune activation was higher after high-level exposure. Increased immune gene expression at the level of receptors and effectors would indicate that the infectious fungal spores were able to breach the host cuticle, cause infection and activate the host’s immunity cascade within 24 h after exposure. We therefore measured the expression of three genes that act at different stages of the immune response, (i) Relish (Rel), a signal cascade gene in the immune deficiency (IMD) pathway [53, 54]; (ii) Prophenoloxidase-activating factor (PPOAF), which starts the process of pathogen melanisation [55,56,57]; and (iii) Defensin (Def), an antimicrobial peptide destroying fungal pathogens [58, 59]. We indeed found that all three genes were significantly upregulated 24 h, but – with the exception of Relish (Fig. S2) – not yet 12 h after exposure to the high pathogen level (Wilcoxon-tests; high dose 12 h: Rel: W = 77, p = 0.045; PPOAF: W = 64, p = 0.267; Def: W = 65, p = 0.267; high dose 24 h: Rel: W = 80, p = 0.009; PPOAF: W = 75, p = 0.027; Def: W = 80, p = 0.009; Fig. 2, S2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Immune gene expression 24 h after exposure to the low or high pathogen dose. Expression of all three measured immune genes (a) Relish, (b) PPOAF, and (c) Defensin was increased in infected males compared to the baseline expression level in healthy males (zero baseline) for males after high-dose (dark green), but not the low-dose (light green) pathogen exposure. For each infected male we show the relative expression level of each gene normalised to the housekeeping gene EF1A, relative to the median gene expression of the respective healthy control males (zero line) as individual data point, median per group as black line and 95% CI as box. Based on a total of 28 males (N = 10 control males, 9 low-dose and 9 high-dose exposed). Significant deviation to the healthy males for each dose and gene indicated by * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ns = non-significant. See Fig. S2 for gene expression 12 h after exposure

In the low-dose exposure, none of the genes was significantly upregulated compared to the baseline gene expression in healthy males (Wilcoxon-tests; low dose 12 h: Rel: W = 58, p = 0.473; PPOAF: W = 49, p = 0.780; Def: W = 50, p = 0.780; low dose 24 h: Rel: W = 65, p = 0.135; PPOAF: W = 50, p = 0.720; Def: W = 68, p = 0.098; Fig. 2, S2). Our data show that C. obscurior fighter males are able to upregulate the expression of their antifungal immune genes > 10-fold within 24 h and therefore show a high immune investment. Mounting such a strong immune response early after exposure could explain the observed survival costs of the males, particularly when under the additional stress of combat.

Discussion

We found that the competitive ability of fighting C. obscurior males was strongly impacted by severe (but not by mild) infection, and that these negative effects already occurred early upon exposure. This is in line with general observations across vertebrates and invertebrate species that both incipient and progressed infection can be costly [13, 14] for males and negatively affect their fighting ability.

High-level infected males were inferior to their healthy rivals and suffered a very high risk of losing the combat and dying, both when entering the fight immediately after exposure to infectious M. robertsii spores, and with an already established infection. At this late infection stage, male morbidity was already quite substantial, since > 50% of the males would not have survived the day even without a fight. However, this was not yet the case during early infection, where male mortality risk more than doubled when confronted with a rival compared to when being kept without an opponent. This suggests a classic fighting-immunity trade-off [14] during early infection, when C. obscurior males strongly invest into raising an immune response. Mounting an immune response is an energetically costly process, involving transcriptional changes in both, immune and metabolic pathways (triggered e.g. by metabolic reprogramming of immune cells) and leading to highly increased glucose consumption [60, 61]. In insects, the link between immunity and metabolism becomes very obvious through the dual role of the fat body, which functions both as a metabolic organ (with an important role in energy storage and metabolic homeostasis [62]) and as key immune organ (producing immune effectors [55]). Even individual proteins play such dual roles, like e.g. the Myocyte Enhancer Factor-2 (Mef2), which, in its phosphorylated state activates gene expression required for anabolic processes. In contrast infection-induced dephosphorylation of Mef2 reduces anabolic processes, but activates the expression of immune effectors like antimicrobial peptides (AMPs) [63]. Similarly, Apolipophorin III (Apo-III), facilitates lipid transfer [64], yet also acts as bacterial and fungal pattern recognition protein and stimulates the host’s encapsulation response [65,66,67]. This interwoven nature of immune pathways and metabolic processes seems to build the basis that infection-induced processes compete for energy allocation with other energy-consuming processes, like growth, energy storage and reproduction [60, 68], which may also explain why immune-challenged individuals can cope less well than healthy individuals with stressful conditions like predator attack [69]. Also in our study, the costs of the males’ immune investment became aggravated when they faced the additional stress of fighting against a rival. Mild infection, on the other hand, resulted in only a minor (non-significant) immune gene upregulation, and males were not inferior in fights against healthy rivals. This is further evidence that resource depletion by mounting an immune response may cause the observed lower competitive success of males when fighting a severe infection.

Males of the social insects, including the social bees, wasps and ants, are not generally expected to be able to raise such a strong immune response as we here describe for Cardiocondyla males (Fig. 2, S2), In fact, studies comparing the immunocompetence between castes found that social insect males have much lower immunocompetence than the colony’s workers or queens [44,45,46]. This is attributed to both genetic and life history effects, as, first, males of haplodiploid species are the haploid sex with lower variability on disease defence loci [70, 71], and, second, their lifespans are often only a few days, where even a good immune defence would not substantially increase life expectancy [72]. The observed high investment into immune defence likely evolved in C. obscurior fighter males due to their uncommonly long lifespan of up to several weeks or even months [41], during which they can engage in multiple (up to approx. 50) matings with the emerging virgin queens [41]. A functioning immune system may therefore be critical for males to ensure surviving at least mild pathogen threats at low cost to their fighting ability and giving them access to many more future mating opportunities. Therefore, we suggest that, even if a high-level infection (which will typically later develop into a fatal disease) induces a trade-off with the male’s fighting ability already at an early infection stage, the benefit of having a functional immune system will outweigh these costs as it allows the males to overcome mild infections without making them inferior competitors. Multiple factors, such as (i) these males not leaving their maternal nest, (ii) C. obscurior ants nesting in arboreal cavities rather than in pathogen-rich soil [29], and (iii) their active performance of hygiene and infection removal behaviours [73], combine to make it likely that contamination of the males with a low pathogen level either via the nest or contagious colony members will be a more common scenario in natural colonies than infection of males with very high doses.

Interestingly, infection levels did not directly impair male aggression, as we could not detect any differences in the aggressive behaviours performed by infected males at any stage or intensity of infection compared to their healthy rivals. This indicates that even the moribund males at the late stage of a severe infection recruited their last resources into fighting their competitors, likely as a terminal investment strategy [26]. However, these males were more aggressed by workers than their healthy rivals, revealing that workers engaged differently into the fights depending on the health state of the competing males. This is in line with inclusive sexual selection [30,31,32], in which workers bias the outcome of male-male competition. A non-exclusive explanation could be that workers attack the fatally-infected males to eliminate them before they die and infectious spores grow out of their cadavers. Such “destructive disinfection” [74] represents an efficient social immunity measure in social insect colonies [33, 75], preventing the development of sporulating cadavers that would otherwise pose a very high risk for disease outbreak in the colony [51, 74].

Workers did not discriminate against males directly after exposure, even if these males carried not yet firmly-attached infectious spores that we found to cross-contaminate the rival male in 45% (9/20) of the fights between high-level exposed and healthy males (N = 20 early stage high load fights analysed for spore transfer). However, this was also not to be expected as aggression in social immunity rarely occurs at the very early stage of pathogen exposure, but rather when infection has established [74]. Even though the males had very close contact during the fights, the contaminated rival carried on average less than 5% of the spore amount of the originally exposed male (1000 vs. 21,000 spores; see Dryad data), which its immune system was likely able to cope with.

Conclusions

Our study revealed that disease negatively impacts male competitive ability in fighting ant males. Interestingly, however, we found that the same outcome – losing the fight – had different causes during the early and late stages of infection. While late-stage males were already moribund at the time of combat and received increased aggression by workers, early-infected males invested heavily into mounting an immune response, which translated into a high mortality when they had to additionally fight a competitor male. Whilst this was true for severe infections, mild infections only led to a non-significant immune activation and did not induce any substantial fighting costs. This shows that the fighting–immunity trade-off is dose-dependent and only occurs at high infection levels, while a well-functioning immune system likely allows males to cope with low-level pathogen exposures in the nest without compromising their fighting ability and allowing future mating success. As being successful in fighting off rivals is key for the males to gain access to female mates, males invested all their energy into fighting, even at terminal disease stages. Under this – and only under this – condition, workers took side and over-proportionally attacked the already moribund male. This inclusive sexual selection by biasing the outcome of male-male competition represents a novel mechanism allowing workers to ensure colony health through social immunity.

Methods

Ant hosts

Colonies of the Myrmicine ant Cardiocondyla obscurior (Wheeler, 1929) were collected in 2013 and 2017 from Tenerife (28.390348 N, -16.596098 E; permits ABSCH-IRCC-ES-237603 ESNC2, Expte: AFF 199/17). 12 stock colonies were reared at constant 27 °C and a day/night light cycle with 14 h of light in the laboratory, and fed twice a week with minced cockroach and were supplied with water and 30% sucrose ad libitum. All experiments were performed at 27 °C. Ant collection, rearing and experimental work was in line with European law and institutional guidelines.

This species shows a male diphenism of wingless fighter males that regularly occur in the nest and engage in deadly fights [6,7,8, 40], as well as peaceful winged disperser males that are produced under stress conditions [76]. Since only the wingless males engage in fighting, we focused on these males in our study. Males were collected as pigmented ‘ready-to-hatch’ pupae from the brood pile and kept with two nurse workers (to help eclosion), until all males were between three and six days old, and their cuticle fully sclerotised when they entered the experiment. We used these age-controlled males to avoid differences between them in their a priori fighting ability, as otherwise old males have a clear fighting benefit over males younger than two days of age, which still possess a soft cuticle [40]. We also size-matched the two rivals per fight, to avoid large body size differences.

Fungal pathogen

As a pathogen, we used the fungus Metarhizium robertsii (strain ARSEF 2575) with an integrated red fluorescent (mRFP1) label [77], obtained from M. Bidochka, Brock University. Metarhizium fungi, incl. M. robertsii, are generalist insect pathogens that cause natural infections in ants [47,48,49]. Their infectious stages, the conidiospores (abbreviated as “spores” throughout), attach to the body surface of their insect hosts, germinate and breach the host cuticle to cause internal infection shortly after. High infection doses cause host death within several days, followed by formation of new infectious particles from the sporulating cadaver [52, 78]. Low-level infections rarely cause disease, and can even induce immune stimulation protecting against future infection [50, 79].

Prior to each experiment, spore suspensions were taken from long-term storage (–80 °C) and cultivated on sabouraud dextrose agar (SDA) at 23 °C for approximately three weeks. Conidiospores were harvested in 0.05% Triton X 100 (Sigma-Aldrich; in autoclaved distilled water). Spore suspensions were counted with a Cellometer Auto M10 counter (Nexcelom) and adjusted to the respective concentrations needed for the experiment (see below). The germination rate of spore suspensions was confirmed to be > 95% before the start of each experiment. To induce infection, males were briefly dipped into the spore suspension and placed on filter paper to absorb excess liquid. Healthy control males were set up in parallel to the fungal treatment in each experiment. They were treated the same way, except that they were only dipped into 0.05% Triton X (sham treatment).

Experimental setup

To determine the effect of early infection, males were dipped into a suspension of either 106 spores/ml (low dose) or 109 spores/ml (high dose), and were placed with five workers (randomly chosen from the same stock colony from outside the brood chamber) and a second male from their colony, all sham-treated. For both the low and high dose, we equally set up control fights, where both males had only received the sham treatment (total 168 early-stage fights, detailed in Table S1). Workers immediately groom off spores from freshly-exposed colony members, so that the number of infectious spores that will then enter the body of the exposed host is drastically reduced [80,81,82]. As spores on a freshly-exposed male are still removable, it is also possible that they cross-contaminate their nestmates [50], including the rival male, during this period.

To obtain late infection stages, we therefore kept each male in isolation for 48 h post exposure before placing them to the workers and rival male. During this period, the spores firmly attach and enter the body to cause internal infection [52], so that males also no longer carry any infectious spores on their body [80] when entering the fight. To prevent these males developing multi-fold higher infection loads due to the lack of spore removal by grooming workers, we used lower exposure doses for induction of these late-stage infections (low dose: 104 spores/ml; high dose: 106 spores/ml). Sham-treated control males were similarly isolated for 48 h, and the workers kept for 48 h in groups of 35 individuals until the start of the fight (total 179 late-stage fights, detailed in Table S1). All fights contained the two rival males and five workers and were performed in experimental containers (height 28 mm, diameter 25 mm) with a plastered and watered base to ascertain the required humidity. All ants per fight replicate (males and workers) originated from the same colony (with 12 different colonies being used as source). The fight outcome was determined 24 h after putting the two males together and with their workers, afterwards all ants were frozen.

We determined the fight outcome, i.e. if the combat remained undecided (both males alive or both males dead), or had a clear winner (the surviving male) and loser (the dead male; ‘decided fights’) for all 347 early and late stage, low and high dose, as well as control fights (see Table S1). 97 out of the 236 high-dose fights and all 111 low-dose fights were further observed for detailed behavioural interactions between the two males and the workers towards the males, for which both males were individually colour-coded before the fight (total N = 208 observed fights). 139 high-dose fights were analysed only for fight outcome, so that males remained unmarked during the fight (with the exception of 12 fights in which males were colour-coded but only fight outcome was analysed). Of these 127 unmarked high-load fight setups, 50 were control fights and 77 contained one infected and one healthy male. 48 of the latter (20 early stage and 28 late stage fights) had a clear winner and a clear looser (i.e. were ‘decided’). To determine, whether the infected males were the winners or losers of these fights, we determined the spore load of both males by PCR at the end of the experiment, and could reliably distinguish between the two males in 40/48 cases, as well as determine the cross-contamination load for the healthy male (see detailed below). Note that the PCR method is not sensitive enough to distinguish low-level infected males from their healthy rivals, which is why all low-load fights were performed with colour-coded males (in addition to all high-load fights, for which we performed behavioural observations, see Table S1). For colour-coding, we applied a tiny dot of metallic email colour (Revell) on the male’s dorsal abdomen (gaster) using a micro dissection needle holder with a fine tungsten needle (1 μm tip diameter), and let the colour dry for multiple hours before start of the experiment. Note that during observation it was unknown to the observer, which colour reflected which male treatment). As per treatment group the proportion of infected individuals being the loser of a fight did not differ between male identification method (PCR vs. colour), we (i) concluded that colour-coding of the males did not alter fight outcome, and (ii) pooled the two methods for analysis of who was the winner or loser in the decided fights. In total, 112 fights ended decided, yet since the PCR method was unable to provide a reliable distinction in eight fights (see above), we could determine the identity of the healthy vs. infected male winning or losing for 104 of the decided fights.

Spore load quantification by ddPCR

In 48 fights of differently-treated males with one male being dead and the other alive, we determined whether it was the infected or the healthy male that had lost the competition. To this end, we froze both rivals after the fight, extracted DNA and ran a droplet digital PCR (ddPCR; Bio-Rad) for direct quantification of their spore load.

For DNA extraction, the samples were homogenized using a TissueLyser II (Qiagen) with a mixture of one 2.8 mm ceramic (Qiagen), five 1 mm zirconia (BioSpec Products) and ~ 100 mg of 425–600 μm glass beads, acid washed (Sigma-Aldrich) in 50 µl of nuclease-free water (Sigma-Aldrich). Homogenization was carried out in two steps (2 × 2 min at 30 Hz). After the first 2 min the tube racks were rotated to ensure uniform disruption and homogenization of all samples. In case the samples were not uniformly crushed yet, the homogenization was repeated. DNA extraction was performed using the DNeasy 96 Blood & Tissue Kit (Qiagen) following the manufacturer’s instructions with a final elution volume of 50 µl. As the used M. robertsii strain (ARSEF 2575) is fluorescently labelled, with each spore carrying a single copy of the mRFP1 gene [77], we could determine absolute spore numbers of our samples by targeting the mRFP1 gene. Primers and probe were designed to bind to the mRFP1 gene (gene bank accession number: KX176868.1;

mRFP1_Forward: 5’-CTGTCCCCTCAGTTCCAGTA, mRFP1_Reverse: 5’- CCGTCCTCGAAGTTCATCAC, mRFP1_probe: 5’[6FAM]AGCACCCCGCCGACATCCCCG[BHQ1]) using Primer3Plus software [83] and were confirmed to only amplify the gene of interest.

The ddPCR reactions with a total volume of 22 µl were prepared as follows: 11 µl of 2x ddPCR Supermix for Probes (Bio-Rad), 900 nM of both primers (Sigma-Aldrich) and 250 nM probe (Sigma-Aldrich), 10 U of both enzymes EcoRI-HF and HindIII-HF (New England Biolabs), 5.27 µl nuclease-free water (Sigma-Aldrich) and 2.2 µl of template DNA. Droplets were generated according to manufacturer’s instructions with the QX200 Droplet Generator (Bio-Rad). The amplification program was initiated with a first step at 95 °C for 10 min to activate the DNA polymerase, followed by 40 cycles of 30 s at 94 °C and 60 s at 56 °C, and finished with 98 °C for 10 min to stop the reaction. For the entire program the ramp rate was set to 2 °C/sec. After amplification droplets were analysed on the QX200 Droplet Reader (Bio-Rad) for the readout of positive and negative droplets.

Data analysis was done using the QuantaSoft Analysis Pro Software (Version 1.0.596; Bio-Rad). The threshold was manually set to 3000. Overloaded samples (low amount of negative droplets) were diluted and re-run. Each run included a H2O negative control (which in 2 of 6 cases showed a single positive droplet). We therefore only considered males that had at least 2 positive droplets in the PCR to be above detection threshold and to have confirmed spore load (either being the exposed male or by cross-contamination from the exposed male). In 8/48 fights we were not able to differentiate between the healthy and infected male as the fungal loads of both males were below detection threshold. These fights could thus not be included in analyses, in which identity of the males was required.

Observation of male and worker aggression

We quantified male and worker aggressive actions by behavioural scan sampling for 97 high-dose and 111 low-dose fights (including their respective control fights of two healthy males, see Table S1; all of these fights included colour-coded males, total N = 208 observed fights). Observers were blind for both fight combination (healthy-healthy vs. healthy-infected) and individual male treatment (colour code did not reveal treatment). A total of 18 scans (each taking one to several seconds) were performed per fight, one every 30 min for the first 8 h and one last one after 24 h, when we also determined fight outcome (note that some fights were still undecided after 24 h). For each male, we determined the number of aggressive interactions performed towards the other male, which included several behaviours like biting, holding (grabbing the rival with the mandibles) and besmearing (male bending its gaster tip to apply hindgut secretion onto the rival). When one of the behaviours occurred alone, the aggression score of the respective scan was 1, when two categories were performed simultaneously, like e.g. holding and besmearing, both were counted, leading to an aggressive score of 2 for that scan. We also counted the number of aggressive interactions that workers performed towards either of the males. Worker aggression included biting, carrying (male is held with the mandibles and carried around), dragging (male is held and pulled over the ground), and dismembering (body parts are intensively attacked or already bitten off). As in the case of male aggression, we counted each behavioural category as one aggression (independently of how many workers performed the same behaviour), leading to aggression scores of > 1 when multiple behavioural categories were performed at the same time (e.g. biting by one and dragging by a second worker). For each male, we then summed over all 18 scans to receive one total score of the aggressive acts it performed towards the other male and one for the aggressive acts it received by the workers.

Determination of male mortality risk in the absence of fighting

To determine the baseline mortality of the infected males (and their healthy controls) in the absence of fighting, we kept males of each infection stage and level with five of its colony’s workers, but no second male, for 24 h and determined their survival. We used a total of 268 males (113 control and 115 infected, as detailed in Table S3).

Activation of the immune system in mild and severe infection

We determined the immune gene activation that males show in the middle and at the end of the fighting period for 57 additional colour-marked males following treatment with the sham solution, the low or the high pathogen dose, and keeping them individually for 12 or 24 h after exposure (N = 9–10 males per time point and dose combination, see figure legends of Fig. 2, S2 for details). For each male, we determined the expression levels of three different immune genes, involved in the cellular and humoral immune response against fungal infection: (i) Relish (Rel) – a transcription factor in the IMD pathway [53, 84], (ii) the shared homolog of the Melanisation Protease 1 and 2 (MP1 and MP2) from Drosophila melanogaster, and the Prophenoloxidase-activating enzyme (PPOAE) from D. mauritiana, which we refer to as Prophenoloxidase-activating factor (PPOAF), a gene that is involved in melanisation as it activates PPO [55,56,57], and (iii) the antimicrobial peptide Defensin (Def) that is activated by the Toll-pathway and destroys fungal cell walls [58, 59]. We used the elongation factor 1-alpha (EF1A) as housekeeping gene [85].

Males were individually snap-frozen in 1.5 ml Safe-Lock Tubes (Eppendorf), their RNA extracted and transcribed to cDNA before performing gene expression analysis using ddPCR. Total RNA was extracted using the Maxwell® RSC instrument together with the Maxwell® simplyRNA Tissue Kit (Promega) according to the manufacturer’s instructions with a final elution volume of 60 µl. Prior to RNA extraction individuals were homogenized in 200 µl homogenization buffer including 4 µl 1-Thioglycerol using a TissueLyser II (Qiagen) with a mixture of five 1 mm zirconia (BioSpec Products) and ~ 100 mg of 425–600 μm glass beads, acid washed (Sigma-Aldrich). Homogenization was carried out in two steps (2 × 2 min at 30 Hz). After the first 2 min the tube racks were rotated to ensure uniform disruption and homogenization of all samples. In case the samples were not uniformly crushed yet, the homogenization was repeated. To ensure the complete removal of residual DNA contamination an additional DNase-I treatment (Sigma-Aldrich) step was performed prior to the reverse transcription. The cDNA synthesis was performed using the iScript cDNA synthesis kit (Bio-Rad) according to manufacturer’s instructions.

To analyse the expression patterns of the immune genes, we used multiplex ddPCR assays, each targeting one immune gene and the housekeeping gene. The EF1A sequence was identified using the primer sequences published in [85]. To identify the target sequences of the immune genes of interest in C. obscurior, as a first step well-annotated protein sequences of Drosophila (Rel - NP_477094.1, PPOAF - XP_033167441, Def - NP_523672.1) were used to identify the respective protein sequences in the two ant species Atta cephalotes (Acep) and Temnothorax longispinosus (Tlon). These two species were chosen because of the annotation status of their genomes and their phylogenetic similarity to C. obscurior. The thereby identified sequences of A. cephalotes (Acep; Rel - XP_012059341.1, PPOAF - XP_012057242.1, Def - XP_012058992.1) and T. longispinosus (Tlon; Rel - TGZ54393.1, PPOAF - TGZ55126.1, Def - TGZ47143.1) were blasted against the Cardiocondyla obscurior (Cobs) genome [86], accessible under GenBank number GCA_019399895.1 (Rel - COBS06226, PPOAF - COBS07801, Def - COS15672). Primers were designed using Primer3Plus [83]) and Multiple Primer Analyzer (https://www.thermofisher.com) software. The following primers and probes were used for the respective genes: EF1A_Forward: 5’- ATTGGAACAGTACCCGTTGG, EF1A_Reverse: 5’-CACCCTTCGGTGGGTTATTT, EF1A_probe: 5’[HEX]ACCTGGTATGGTCGTTACCTTTGCACCCGT[BHQ1]; Relish: Rel_Forward: 5’-ACGGATTTAGGATGGACACC, Rel_Reverse: 5’- TTGGTGGCTTCCTTCAACA, Rel_probe: 5’[6FAM]TGCTCTCTTGTGCAGACTGGCGCAGA[BHQ1]; PPOAF: PPOAF_Forward: 5’- TGCTGCTCACTGTATCAAGG, PPOAF_Reverse: 5’- TCTGTTTCAGTGTCGGTGTC, PPOAF_probe: 5’ [6FAM]ACTGGCGTCTGACCAGCGTCCGT[BHQ1]; Defensin: Def_Forward: 5’- ACGGGCCTACTTACGAATTG, Def_Reverse: 5’- CGCAAGCACTATGGTTGATG, Def_probe: 5’[6FAM]CGAAGAGGAGCCGTCACACCTGACGC[BHQ1].

The ddPCR reactions with a total volume of 22 µl were prepared as follows: 11 µl of 2x ddPCR Supermix for Probes (Bio-Rad), 900 nM of each primer (Sigma-Aldrich) for the immune gene of interest as well as the housekeeping gene and 250 nM of the respective probes (Sigma-Aldrich), 3.74 µl nuclease-free water (Sigma-Aldrich) and 2.2 µl of cDNA. The cDNA was sonicated (15 s) before being added to the reagent mix. All further steps were performed as described above, except that the amplification program was slightly modified to improve the separation between positive and negative droplets. The amplification program was initiated with a first step at 95 °C for 10 min, followed by 50 cycles of 30 s at 94 °C and 2 min at 56 °C, and finished with 98 °C for 10 min. All PCR steps were carried out with a ramp rate of 1 °C/sec. The thresholds were set manually as follows: EF1A 4000, PPOAF 1500, Rel 2000 and Def 4000. Immune gene expression levels were normalized to the reference gene EF1A.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed in the program ‘R’ version 4.0.3 [87]. We tested the difference between the two fight combinations (healthy-healthy vs healthy-infected) separately for each scenario (early stage low and high dose, late stage high and low dose). This was both due to (i) our experimental design, as a 48 h isolation period was required in the late but not the early infection treatment, and (ii) experimental constraints, since males only emerged from their colonies over multiple months, given that 694 males were required for the 347 fights, another 268 for the determination of male mortality risk in the absence of fighting and further 57 for the immune gene expression, totalling to 1019 males. We could therefore run simultaneous experiments of healthy-healthy and healthy-infected combinations of the same infection stage and dose, but not across all treatments. For statistical analysis, we applied Chi-Square tests (or Fisher’s exact tests when expected frequencies were < 5) or generalized mixed models (GLMM) (‘lme 4’ [88]). We assessed model assumptions (residual normality and heterogeneity and no overdispersion) with ‘DHARMa’ [89] and checked model stability and the presence of influential data points (Cook’s distance and dfbetas). Whenever multiple inferences were made from the same dataset, we corrected the overall p-values for multiple testing with the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure to protect against a false discovery rate of 0.05 [90]. We report two-sided, corrected p-values. All graphs were made using the ‘ggplot2’ package [91].

Fight outcome

To determine whether the fight outcome (one male dead, both males dead, or both males alive) differed between fight combinations (healthy-healthy vs. healthy-infected) for each scenario (early stage low and high dose, as well as late stage high and low dose), we ran logistic regressions with binomial error term and logit-link function for each outcome and implemented fight outcome as response variable and fight combination as predictor variable (multiple testing adjustment for 3 comparisons each; Fig. 1a,b, Table S2) and included ‘colony’ as a random effect. To further test for the decided fights whether early- or late-stage infected males were losing more often against the healthy rival than expected by chance, we used Chi-Square tests or, if the minimum expected frequency was less than 5, Fisher’s exact tests (Fig. 1a,b).

Male aggression performed towards its rival

For each of the 112 observed fights with one healthy and one infected male (see Table S1), we summed the number of male aggressive behaviours observed against its rival as detailed above and analysed them as count data. For each fight scenario (early stage low and high dose, late stage high and low dose), we ran GLMMs with negative binomial error term and log-link function as the data was over-dispersed, with male infection status (healthy vs. infected) as predictor and the number of male aggressive acts as response. ‘Colony’ and ‘fight replicate’ were included as random effects (multiple testing adjustment for 2 comparisons each). While the statistics are performed with the raw data, we visualise the infected male’s aggression in comparison to its healthy rival (by subtracting the infected male’s aggression score from that of the healthy male for each fight; Fig. 1c,d). We ran the same models also for the control fights between the two healthy males (N = 46 early and N = 50 late control fights, Table S1; low and high doses pooled, as both males in the control fights had only received sham treatment across dosages), by randomly assigning the two males per fight into either male A or male B, and iterating the model 100 times. None of the models showed a significant difference between male aggression of males categorized randomly into A and B males, confirming that male aggression was also balanced in the control fights.

Worker aggression towards males

For each of the 112 observed fights with one healthy and one infected male we further summed the number of worker aggressive acts against each male as detailed above (count data). To test if the infected vs. healthy males received a different level of aggression by the workers, we ran GLMMs with Poisson error terms and log-link function with male infection status (healthy vs. infected) as predictor and the number of worker attacks as response. ‘Colony’ and ‘fight replicate’ were included as random effects (multiple testing adjustment for 2 comparisons each). Statistics were run on the raw data, while we visualise the difference of worker aggression towards the infected males compared to that towards the healthy male of the same fight by subtracting the aggression towards the infected male from that directed towards the healthy male (Fig. 1e,f). Again, we ran the same models also for the 46 early and 50 late control fights, with randomized categorisation of the two males per fight into male A or B, and 100 times iteration of running the models, and corrected for multiple testing. For the early fights, 2/100 iterations showed a significant difference between worker aggression received by male A vs. B. In the late fights, 16/100 iterations showed imbalanced worker aggression, which was mostly due to one influential fight, in which workers aggressed one of the males –the later loser– 21-fold more than in the other control fights (15 attacks vs. a mean ± SD of 0.69 ± 1.36 attacks per male in all remaining late control fights ), with always the category A or B being more aggressed to which this highly-attacked male was randomly assigned to (in our set of iterations 41% A and 59% B). Exclusion of this special fight led to only 7/100 of the iterations showing significant imbalance of worker aggression. Similar to fights between differentially-aged males [40], over-proportional worker aggression against the later loser was therefore also possible in fights between the two similarly-aged healthy males in our experiment, yet without any predictable pattern between males A and B.

Male mortality in the absence of fighting

To determine if the baseline risk of the males to die during the 24 h fight period in the absence of fighting was higher for the infected males than their respective healthy male control, we ran a logistic regression with binomial error term and logit-link function with male survival (dead vs. alive) as response and male infection status (healthy vs. infected) as predictor for each dose and stage of infection (Fig. 1 g,h, S1; Table S3).

Costs of fighting

To determine the additional costs of fighting of infected males, we compared the survival of non-fighters (baseline risk) to the survival of fighting males for each dose and infection stage. We considered fights with one healthy and one infected male that either ended decided (one dead) or both males being dead and calculated the proportion in which the infected males were dying and compared it to the proportion of male mortality in the absence of fighting using Chi-Square tests or, if the minimum expected frequency was less than 5, Fisher’s exact tests (Fig. 1 g,h; multiple testing adjustment for 4 comparisons each).

Immune gene activation

To test whether the gene expression differed between infected and healthy males at 12 and 24 h after exposure for both the low and high dose, we compared their normalised gene expression values (value of immune gene / value of housekeeping gene) by Wilcoxon-tests for independent samples (multiple testing adjustment for 6 comparisons). For visualisation (Fig. 2, S2), we show the difference of the infected males to their respective healthy male control, by subtracting the median of the healthy males from each infected male.

Data Availability

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available in the Dryad data repository; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.5061/dryad.bk3j9kdhb.

References

  1. Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Gibson RM, Guinness FE. The logical stag: adaptive aspects of fighting in red deer (Cervus elaphus L). Anim Behav. 1979;27(PART 1):211–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Schnell AK, Smith CL, Hanlon RT, Harcourt R. Giant australian cuttlefish use mutual assessment to resolve male-male contests. Anim Behav. 2015;107:31–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Kemp DJ, Wiklund C. Fighting without weaponry: a review of male-male contest competition in butterflies. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2001;49(6):429–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. West SA, Murray MG, Machado CA, Griffin AS, Herre EA. Testing Hamilton’s rule with competition between relatives. Nature. 2001;409(6819):510–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Abe J, Kamimura Y, Shimada M. Individual sex ratios and offspring emergence patterns in a parasitoid wasp, Melittobia australica (Eulophidae), with superparasitism and lethal combat among sons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2005;57(4):366–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Heinze J, Hölldobler B, Yamauchi K. Male competition in Cardiocondyla ants. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1998;42(4):239–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Kinomura K, Yamauchi K. Fighting and mating behaviors of dimorphic males in the ant - Cardiocondyla wroughtoni. J Ethol. 1987;5(1):75–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Heinze J, Hölldobler B. Fighting for a harem of queens: physiology of reproduction in Cardiocondyla male ants. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90(18):8412–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. The ants. Harvard University Press; 1990.

  10. Yamauchi K, Kimura Y, Corbara B, Kinomura K, Tsuji K. Dimorphic ergatoid males and their reproductive behavior in the ponerine ant Hypoponera bondroiti. Ins Soc. 1996;43:119–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Hamilton WD. Wingless and fighting males in fig wasps and other insects. In: Eds Blum MS, Blum NA, editors. Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects. New York: Academic Press; 1979. pp. 167–220.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Boomsma JJ, Baer B, Heinze J. The evolution of male traits in social insects. Annu Rev Entomol. 2005;50:395–420.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Sheldon BC, Verhulst S. Ecological immunology: costly parasite defences and trade-offs in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 1996;11(8):317–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Schwenke RA, Lazzaro BP, Wolfner MF. Reproduction-Immunity Trade-Offs in Insects. Annu Rev Entomol. 2016;61:239–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Barribeau SM, Otti O. Sexual Reproduction and Immunity. eLS. 2020;1–10.

  16. Mazur A, Booth A. Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav Brain Sci. 1998;21(3):353–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Eisenegger C, Haushofer J, Fehr E. The role of testosterone in social interaction. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(6):263–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Alexander J, Stimson WH. Sex hormones and the course of parasitic infection. Parasitol Today. 1988;4(7):189–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Zuk M, Thornhill R, Ligon JD, Johnson K. Parasites and mate choice in red jungle fowl. Integr Comp Biol. 1990;30(2):235–44.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Hillgarth N, Wingfield JC. Testosterone and immunosuppression in vertebrates: implications for parasite-mediated sexual selection. Parasites Pathog. 1997;143–55.

  21. Hart BL. Biological basis of the behavior of sick animals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1988;12(2):123–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kent S, Bluthé RM, Kelley KW, Dantzer R. Sickness behavior as a new target for drug development. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1992;13:24–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Konsman JP, Parnet P, Dantzer R. Cytokine-induced sickness behavior: mechanisms and implications. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25(3):154–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Shakhar K, Shakhar G. Why do we feel sick when infected—can Altruism play a role? PLoS Biol. 2015;13(10).

  25. Liu PC, Wei JR, Tian S, Hao D. Male-male lethal combat in the quasi-gregarious parasitoid Anastatus disparis (Hymenoptera: Eupelmidae). Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Clutton-Brock TH. Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals. Am Nat. 1984;123(2):212–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Loehle C. Social barriers to pathogen transmission in wild animal populations. Ecology. 1995;76(2):326–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Konrad M, Pull CD, Metzler S, Seif K, Naderlinger E, Grasse AV, et al. Ants avoid superinfections by performing risk-adjusted sanitary care. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115(11):2782–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Heinze J, Cremer S, Eckl N, Schrempf A. Stealthy invaders: the biology of Cardiocondyla tramp ants. Insectes Soc. 2006;53(1):1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Sunamura E, Hoshizaki S, Sakamoto H, Fujii T, Nishisue K, Suzuki S, et al. Workers select mates for queens: a possible mechanism of gene flow restriction between supercolonies of the invasive Argentine ant. Naturwissenschaften. 2011;98(5):361–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Helft F, Monnin T, Doums C. First evidence of inclusive sexual selection in the ant Cataglyphis cursor: worker aggressions differentially affect male access to virgin queens. Ethology. 2015;121(7):641–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Vidal M, Königseder F, Giehr J, Schrempf A, Lucas C, Heinze J. Worker ants promote outbreeding by transporting young queens to alien nests. Commun Biol. 2021;4(1):1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Cremer S, Armitage SAO, Schmid-Hempel P. Social immunity. Curr Biol. 2007;17(16):R693–702.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Hamilton WD, Zuk M. Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites? Science. 1982;218(4570):384–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Holway DA, Lach L, Suarez AV, Tsutsui ND, Case TJ. The causes and consequences of ant invasions. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 2002;33:181–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Cremer S, Ugelvig LV, Drijfhout FP, Schlick-Steiner BC, Florian M, Seifert B, et al. The evolution of invasiveness in Garden Ants. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(12):e3838.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Kenis M, Auger-Rozenberg MA, Roques A, Timms L, Péré C, Cock MJW, et al. Ecological effects of invasive alien insects. Biol Invasions. 2009;11(1):21–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Heinze J. Life-history evolution in ants: the case of Cardiocondyla. Proc R Soc B. 2017;284(1850).

  39. Yamauchi K, Kawase N. Pheromonal manipulation of workers by a fighting male to kill his rival males in the ant Cardiocondyla wroughtonii. Naturwissenschaften. 1992;79(6):274–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Cremer S, Suefuji M, Schrempf A, Heinze J. The dynamics of male-male competition in Cardiocondyla obscurior ants. BMC Ecol. 2012;12(7):11–5.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Metzler S, Heinze J, Schrempf A. Mating and longevity in ant males. Ecol Evol. 2016;6(24):8903–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Schrempf A, Moser A, Delabie J, Heinze J. Sperm traits differ between winged and wingless males of the ant Cardiocondyla obscurior. Integr Zool. 2016;11(6):427–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Boomsma JJ. Beyond promiscuity: mate-choice commitments in social breeding. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2013;368:1613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Baer B, Krug A, Boomsma JJ, Hughes WOH. Examination of the immune responses of males and workers of the leaf-cutting ant Acromyrmex echinatior and the effect of infection. Insectes Soc. 2005;52:298–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Vainio L, Hakkarainen H, Rantala MJ, Sorvari J. Individual variation in immune function in the ant Formica exsecta; effects of the nest, body size and sex. Evol Ecol. 2004;18(1):75–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Gerloff CU, Ottmer BK, Schmid-Hempel P. Effects of inbreeding on immune response and body size in a social insect, Bombus terrestris. Func Ecol. 2003;17(5):582–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Angelone S, Bidochka MJ. Diversity and abundance of entomopathogenic fungi at ant colonies. J Invertebr Pathol. 2018;156:73–6.

  48. Casillas-Pérez B, Pull CD, Naiser F, Naderlinger E, Matas J, Cremer S. Early queen infection shapes developmental dynamics and induces long-term disease protection in incipient ant colonies. Ecol Lett. 2022;25(1):89–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Pull CD, Hughes WOH, Brown MJF. Tolerating an infection: an indirect benefit of co-founding queen associations in the ant Lasius niger. Naturwissenschaften. 2013;100(12):1125–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Konrad M, Vyleta ML, Theis FJ, Stock M, Tragust S, Klatt M, et al. Social transfer of pathogenic fungus promotes active immunisation in ant colonies. PLOS Biol. 2012;10(4):e1001300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Hughes WOH, Eilenberg J, Boomsma JJ. Trade-offs in group living: transmission and disease resistance in leaf-cutting ants. Proc R Soc B. 2002;269:1811–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Vestergaard S, Butt TM, Bresciani J, Gillespie AT, Eilenberg J. Light and Electron Microscopy Studies of the infection of the Western Flower Thrips Frankliniella occidentalis (Thysanoptera: Thripidae) by the Entomopathogenic Fungus Metarhizium anisopliae. J Invertebr Pathol. 1999;73(1):25–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Myllymäki H, Valanne S, Rämet M. The Drosophila Imd Signaling Pathway. J Immunol. 2014;192(8):3455–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Sheehan G, Garvey A, Croke M, Kavanagh K. Innate humoral immune defences in mammals and insects: the same, with differences? Virulence. 2018;9(1):1625–39.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Gillespie JP, Kanost MR, Trenczek T. Biological mediators of insect immunity. Annu Rev Entomol. 1997;42:611–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Cerenius L, Lee BL, Söderhäll K. The proPO-system: pros and cons for its role in invertebrate immunity. Trends Immunol. 2008;29(6):263–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Cerenius L, Söderhäll K. The prophenoloxidase-activating system in invertebrates. Immunol Rev. 2004;198:116–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Viljakainen L, Pamilo P. Identification and molecular characterization of defensin gene from the ant Formica aquilonia. Insect Mol Biol. 2005;14(4):335–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Viljakainen L, Pamilo P. Selection on an antimicrobial peptide defensin in ants. J Mol Evol. 2008;67(6):643–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Bajgar A, Kucerova K, Jonatova L, Tomcala A, Schneedorferova I, Okrouhlik J, et al. Extracellular adenosine mediates a systemic metabolic switch during immune response. PloS Biol. 2015;13(4):e1002135.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Schlamp F, Delbare SYN, Early AM, Wells MT, Basu S, Clark AG. Dense time-course gene expression profiling of the Drosophila melanogaster innate immune response. BMC Genomics. 2021;22(1):304.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Dolezal T, Krejcova GK, Bajgar A, Nedbalova P, Strasser P. Molecular regulations of metabolism during immune response in insects. Insect Biochem Mol Biol. 2019;109:31–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Clark RI, Tan SWS, Péan CB, Roostalu U, Vivancos V, Bronda K, et al. MEF2 is an in vivo Immune-Metabolic switch. Cell. 2013;155(2):435–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Blacklock BJ, Ryan RO. Hemolymph lipid transport. Insect Biochem Mol Biol. 1994;24(9):885–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Dhawan R, Gupta K, Kajla M, Kakani P, Choudhury TP, Kumar S, et al. Apolipophorin-III acts as a positive regulator of Plasmodium development in Anopheles stephensi. Front Physiol. 2017;8:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Whitten MMA, Tew IF, Lee BL, Ratcliffe NA. A novel role for an insect apolipoprotein (apolipophorin III) in β-1,3-glucan pattern recognition and cellular encapsulation reactions. J Immunol. 2004;172(4):2177–85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Yu HZ, Wang J, Zhang SZ, Toufeeq S, Li B, Li Z, et al. Molecular characterisation of Apolipophorin-III gene in Samia cynthia ricini and its roles in response to bacterial infection. J Invertebr Pathol. 2018;159:61–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Gray EM, Bradley TJ. Malarial infection in Aedes aegypti: effects on feeding, fecundity and metabolic rate. Parasitology. 2006;132(Pt 2):169–7.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Otti O, Gantenbein-Ritter I, Jacot A, Brinkhof MWG. Immune response increases predation risk. Evolution. 2012;66(3):732–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. O’Donnell S, Beshers SN. The role of male disease susceptibility in the evolution of haplodiploid insect societies. Proc R Soc B. 2004;271(1542):979–83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Ruiz-Gonzáles MX, Brown MJ. Males vs workers: testing the assumptions of the haploid susceptibility hypothesis in bumblebees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60:501–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Stürup M, Baer B, Boomsma JJ. Short independent lives and selection for maximal sperm survival make investment in immune defences unprofitable for leaf-cutting ant males. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2014;68:947–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Ugelvig LV, Kronauer DJC, Schrempf A, Heinze J, Cremer S. Rapid anti-pathogen response in ant societies relies on high genetic diversity. Proc R Soc B. 2010;277(1695):2821–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Pull CD, Brown MJ, Grasse AV, Schmitt T, Wiesenhofer F, Tragust S, et al. Destructive disinfection of infected brood prevents systemic disease spread in ant colonies. eLlife. 2018;7:e32073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Cremer S. Social immunity in insects. Curr Biol. 2019;29(11):R458–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Cremer S, Heinze J. Stress grows wings: environmental induction of winged dispersal males in Cardiocondyla ants. Curr Biol. 2003;13(3):219–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Fang W, Pei Y, Bidochka MJ. Transformation of Metarhizium anisopliae mediated by Agrobacterium tumefaciens. Can J Microbiol. 2006;52(7):623–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Hajek AE, St Leger RJ. Interactions between fungal pathogens and insect hosts. Annu Rev Entomol. 1994;39(1):293–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Liu L, Li G, Sun P, Lei C, Huang Q. Experimental verification and molecular basis of active immunization against fungal pathogens in termites. Sci Rep. 2015;5:15106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Walker TN, Hughes WOH. Adaptive social immunity in leaf-cutting ants. Biol Lett. 2009;5(4):446–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Reber A, Purcell J, Buechel SD, Buri P, Chapuisat M. The expression and impact of antifungal grooming in ants. J Evol Biol. 2011;24:954–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Rosengaus RB, Traniello JFA, Lefebvre ML, Carlock DM. The social transmission of disease between adult male and female reproductives of the dampwood termite Zootermopsis angusticollis. Ethol Ecol Evol. 2000;12(4):419–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Untergasser A, Nijveen H, Rao X, Bisseling T. Primer3Plus, an enhanced web interface to Primer3. Nucleic Acids Res. 2007;35:71–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Sheehan G, Farrell G, Kavanagh K. Immune priming: the secret weapon of the insect world. Virulence. 2020;11(1):238–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Klein A, Schrader L, Gil R, Manzano-Marín A, Flórez L, Wheeler D, et al. A novel intracellular mutualistic bacterium in the invasive ant Cardiocondyla obscurior. ISME J. 2016;10:376–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Errbii M, Keilwagen J, Hoff KJ, Steffen R, Altmüller J, Oettler J, et al. Transposable elements and introgression introduce genetic variation in the invasive ant Cardiocondyla obscurior. Mol Ecol. 2021;30(23):6211–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2020. https://www.R-project.org.

  88. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67(1).

  89. Hartig F, DHARMa. Residual Diagnostics for Hierarchical Regression Models. Compr R Arch Netw. 2020;1–26.

  90. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false Discovery rate: a practical and powerful Approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B. 1995;57(1):289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Wickham H, Chang W, Henry L, Pedersen TL, Takahashi K, Wilke C et al. ggplot2: Create Elegant Data Visualisations Using the Grammar of Graphics. 2018.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to Mike Bidochka for the fungal strain, Lukas Schrader for sharing the C. obscurior genome data for primer development, the Lab Support Facility of ISTA for general laboratory support and help with the permit approval procedures, and the Finca El Quinto for letting us collect ants on their property. We thank the Social Immunity Team at ISTA for help with ant collection and experimental help, in particular Elina Hanhimäki and Marta Gorecka for behavioural observation, and Elisabeth Naderlinger for spore load PCRs. We further thank the Social Immunity Team and Jürgen Heinze for continued discussion and comments on the manuscript.

Funding

Open access funding provided by Institute of Science and Technology Austria (ISTA). This project received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement No 771402 to SC). The funding body had no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SM and SC conceived the study. The experiments were performed by SM. JK and AVG established the molecular methods. JK collected the data for the immune gene expression. SM curated and analysed all data, with JK performing the randomization statistics. SM prepared the figures after joint conception with SC. SM and SC wrote the manuscript, with input from AVG and JK. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sylvia Cremer.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

C. obscurior is an unprotected invasive species. Ant collection followed the rules for Access and Benefit-Sharing following the Nagoya protocol and was granted by the Spanish Ministry of Agriculture, Fisheries and Environment (ABSCH-IRCC-ES-237603 ESNC2), as well as the Council of Tenerife (Expte: AFF 199/17). All experimental work is in line with European law and institutional guidelines.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Supplementary Material 2

Supplementary Material 3

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Metzler, S., Kirchner, J., Grasse, A.V. et al. Trade-offs between immunity and competitive ability in fighting ant males. BMC Ecol Evo 23, 37 (2023). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-023-02137-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-023-02137-7

Keywords